- Research
- Open access
- Published:
Identifying mediators of healthy lifestyle adoption after stroke: a focus group study using a Theoretical Domains Framework guided analysis
BMC Neurology volume 25, Article number: 135 (2025)
Abstract
Background
Lifestyle measures in addition to pharmacotherapy are recommended to optimise stroke secondary prevention. Adopting and sustaining good health behaviours after stroke necessitates ongoing motivation, influenced by complex social and cultural factors. This study analysed stroke survivors’ experiences of addressing their lifestyle-related risks through a comprehensive theoretical lens addressing cognitive, affective, social, and environmental influences. Patient and public involvement (PPI) enhanced the research quality and transparency.
Methods
Eight focus group discussions (N = 35 stroke participants; N = 3 family members/informal carers) were facilitated using semi-structured questions co-developed with a PPI panel. Purposive sampling ensured adequate representation (e.g. urban/rural location and stroke-related disabilities). Data were first coded and categorised inductively and mapped to the Theoretical Domains Framework (TDF) deductively to identify relevant constructs and theories of behaviour-change.
Results
Participants reported risk reducing lifestyle changes as largely self-directed activities they figured out themselves. Their experiences mapped to 10 of the 14 theoretical domains of the TDF. The most reported behaviour-change mediators discussed were in the domains of Knowledge and Social Influences, seen as encouraging change and supporting emotional reactions. Goals were discussed in a limited way indicating underutilisation. Reminders, reinforcements, and rules to observe for maintaining healthy behaviours, mapping to the Reinforcement and Behavioural Regulation domains, were valued constructs. Psychosocial challenges, emotional responses and cognitive difficulties (Memory, Attention & Decision Processes and Emotions domains) were strongly evident, resonating with the experiences of our PPI contributor and interfacing with behaviour change processes and knowledge uptake. Health-beliefs, self-identity and perceived ability to change behaviour were considered to assert both positive and negative influences on behaviours, mapping to Social/Professional Role & Identity, Beliefs about Consequences and Beliefs about Capabilities domains. ‘Know how’ was highlighted as largely lacking for behaviour change, with the associated theoretical domains Intentions, Skills, Environmental context & resources to encourage skills development and Optimism about change notably absent from discussions.
Conclusions
The TDF proved a valuable tool to link stroke survivors’ secondary prevention experiences and unmet needs with recognised constructs for behaviour-change. Results have important theory-driven implications to guide future interventions designed to support individuals in risk reducing behaviours following stroke.
Introduction
Modifiable risk factors account for up to 90% of all strokes at a population level, the majority of which are amenable to lifestyle modification [1]. As stroke survival rates increase, the burden of recurrent stroke also increases [2]. Recurrent strokes are associated with higher mortality rates and greater disability levels [3, 4], making secondary prevention an escalating priority [5]. Secondary prevention guidelines identify that all modifiable risk factors for recurrence need to be targeted and managed well for optimised secondary prevention [6,7,8], evermore so, as reductions in recurrence rates related to pharmacological management have largely plateaued over the past decade [9]. Adherence with recommended lifestyle-related risk reducing behaviours after stroke remains low [10, 11], and presents a clear target for secondary prevention. Stroke secondary prevention was defined by Delphi consensus as a strategy that “supports and improves long-term health and well-being in everyday life and reduces the risk of another stroke, by drawing from a spectrum of theoretically informed interventions and educational strategies” [12]. Stroke secondary prevention guidelines further recognise this need for proven theoretical models and behaviour change techniques to support health behaviours, noting advice alone is insufficient to affect positive change [7].
Changing any health-related behaviour for the better is a dynamic process which is constantly evolving [13]. Adopting and sustaining individualised health behaviours, while a key strategy in stroke secondary prevention [14], is additionally challenging as it necessitates ongoing motivation, and is influenced by complex social settings and cultural factors [15, 16]. Moreover, supports provided to promote change need to be guided by established theories and strategies rooted in evidence-based practices [12, 17]. In delivering complex interventions for stroke secondary prevention, healthcare professionals are required to better understand behaviour-change processes and the mediators by which to affect behaviour change [18]. Low to moderate certainty evidence supports behaviour change interventions after stroke as effective in reducing future cardiac events, reducing hypertension, improving physical activity participation and medication adherence, and reducing post-stroke depression [19,20,21,22,23]. However, a recent overview of reviews highlighted limited theory-based research and use of behaviour-change mediators in stroke secondary prevention complex intervention trials [19, 24].
Qualitative research has previously examined peoples’ experiences of receiving stroke secondary prevention information and engaging in risk reducing activities [25,26,27,28,29,30]. Physical barriers such as pain, mobility, and fatigue; mental/emotional barriers including anxiety, fear, cognitive problems; lack of clear information provision; poor access to social supports and environmental factors were reported impediments to adopting and sustaining healthy behaviours post-stroke [15, 26,27,28, 31]. Perceptions of health information messaging after stroke were formerly considered under the theory of planned behaviour, where confusing or contradictory advice regarding healthy lifestyle behaviour was seen to negatively influence behavioural intentions [26]. Family members and peers were noted to exert either positive or negative influences on behavioural patterns, and the influence of healthcare professionals on behavioural intentions was rarely perceived. A meta-synthesis of qualitative research addressing secondary prevention group-based interventions, from the perspective of stroke survivors and family members, identified benefits that included feeling supported by others with shared understanding and by knowledgeable health professionals, and acquiring new knowledge and gaining confidence, which indirectly speak to mediators for behaviour change [32].
No study to date, however, has explicitly mapped individuals’ experiences and perceptions of their risk reducing activities after stroke to theories or theoretical frameworks of behaviour change and their constructs. Therefore, this study aimed to better understand how individuals address lifestyle related risk factors along their stroke journey, identifying enablers for any positive behaviour changes made, and barriers and unmet needs related to ongoing risks. To better understand these experiences in the context of behaviour change theory, focus group discussion results were mapped to the Theoretical Domains Framework (TDF) [33], a comprehensive psychological model used to identify and understand factors influencing health behaviour change. It encompasses a range of theoretical domains such as knowledge, beliefs, and social influences [34]. New knowledge generated in this study about helpful or underutilised behaviour change constructs should help to inform future content, delivery and reporting of complex interventions addressing stroke secondary prevention.
Patient and public involvement (PPI), defined as “research carried out ‘with’ or ‘by’ members of the public rather than ‘to’, ‘about’ or ‘for’ them” [35] was embedded in this study. To enhance quality and transparency, throughout the research process, this study aimed to actively involve a PPI panel of stroke champions, purposely assembled to represent the lived experience of stroke. PPI champions collaborated as research partners at all stages from study development to interpretation and synthesis of findings. Their perspectives are important to guide and inform future secondary prevention intervention development.
Methods
Study design
A qualitative exploration study addressing people’s experiences adopting personal risk reducing behaviours after stroke. Consolidated criteria for reporting qualitative research (COREQ) guidelines [36] were followed (Additional file 1). Data were collected using focus group discussions(FGD). This methodology, used successfully with stroke survivors [26, 37, 38], facilitates exploration of the participants’ experiences, feelings, and ideas and provides an opportunity to encourage and observe interaction amongst participants [39]. Interpretation of the shared and compared experiences of importance to people after stroke should help contribute to a better understanding of healthy behaviour adoption.
Research team and reflexivity
The research team comprised a primary investigator (PI), an experienced cardiovascular nurse specialist and researcher with prior experience of delivering stroke secondary prevention interventions (N = 1 female, PH); an early-stage researcher from a middle income country undergoing mentoring in research methods (N = 1 female, PP); and a senior academic, experienced in qualitative research methods and stroke service delivery (N = 1 female, OL). While the study PI who conducted the focus-group discussions, was an independent researcher undertaking the study as part of academic endeavour to PhD, she had worked in both stroke support organisations previously, providing education and facilitating stroke support groups.
Participants and recruitment
Community-based Stroke Support Groups represent individuals and families after stroke living in the community. They meet on a monthly basis, facilitated by the voluntary organisations, to socialise, learn more about stroke and give and receive peer support.
Recruitment flyers and information leaflets (Additional file 2) were shared by the voluntary organisations supporting the research with their Stroke Support Group membership. Subsequent to this, the researcher visited each interested group to provide more detailed information and suggest potential focus group dates (Fig. 1). Both individuals after stroke and family carers were welcome and mixed participation in the groups was anticipated. This would ensure broad demographic inclusion in age, gender, urban/rural, socio-demographic status, range of stage and ability post-stroke. The intention was to host four to six FGDs each with approximately 6/7 participants to stimulate group discussion, ensure sufficient rich data was generated that could identify issues and provide broad understanding, as guided by the literature [39].
Purposive sampling ensured a mix of urban and rural areas, which included deprived and affluent areas, were represented. Community dwelling adults > 18 years of age, at least 6 months post stroke were eligible to take part. Individuals with mobility and/or communication impairment were eligible to participate if they had capacity to attend an hour-long discussion with other stroke survivors. To maximise inclusivity, a specially facilitated focus group with speech and language therapy support was provided as an option for people with post-stroke aphasia. Exclusion criteria included individuals with severe receptive aphasia; significant cognitive impairment limiting capacity to understand the study or the consent process; acute or unstable illness. Recruitment of family members/informal carers occurred directly when in attendance at stroke support group meetings. The inclusion of informal caregivers in the focus groups provided an alternative perspective from family/caregiver experiences. Stroke survivors and family members/informal carers, volunteers by self-selection, who expressed interest in participating were given a participant information leaflet (which included a QR code link to an online presentation in an accessible format) and a consent form. Informed consent was provided by participants prior to attending FGDs.
Data collection
Each FGD was guided by a semi-structured question schedule to explore experiences and perceptions of where and how lifestyle related risk factors were addressed along their stroke journey, and any identified barriers and facilitators to adopting personalised risk reduction behaviours. The topic guide was developed and reviewed with the project’s PPI panel contributors (Additional file 3) and questions were asked in a conversational manner. The focus group facilitator introduced herself, explained the purpose of each focus group, establishing a rapport with participants and making sure they understood the reason for the focus groups. Four main areas were identified to explore:
-
Knowledge of stroke risk factors in relation to lifestyle behaviours/daily habits or activities.
-
Whether planning for healthy living after stroke was discussed and with whom.
-
Perceived ease/difficulty in changing lifestyle behaviours.
-
Barriers and facilitators to maintaining behaviour change.
All FGDs were one hour in duration and were conducted by the PI (PH) with field notes recorded after each session. A second moderator attended the first three FGD to observe and take notes until the primary researcher was comfortable nothing was being missed. The focus groups were held in-person in the support groups’ usual meeting places (N = 7) or on-line using virtual conferencing on Zoom (N = 1), between September and December 2022. The discussions were digitally recorded, transcribed verbatim and pseudo-anonymised.
Data analysis
Analysis employed a framework approach [40], moving from the inductive in the first instance to the deductive, where descriptions of accounts and experiences were reflected and described in relation to the fourteen domains of the TDF. The TDF synthesises 33 theories of behaviour change and their constructs, into groupings that make behaviour-change theories more accessible. Comprising 87 component parts across fourteen overarching domains, the TDF covers a breath of relevant constructs and lowers the risk of constructs being missed with the use of a single theory [34](Additional file 4). This integrative and flexible framework (TDF) is documented as allowing comprehensive appraisal of behavioural constructs in qualitative studies, with an inductive aspect to analysis recommended [33].
Inductive analysis commenced by repeated listening to the focus group recordings, allowing immersion in the data as a whole, and transcribed verbatim by the primary researcher (PH). Each transcript was read and re-read multiple times and associated field notes checked for further insights to gain a sense of the data as a whole. Transcripts were then open-coded line by line independently by two researchers (PH and PP) in a qualitative content analysis approach to identify initial meaning units that related to healthy lifestyle adoption that were then coded from the raw data [41]. Following detailed examination, constant comparison and discussion between coders (PH and PP) occurred to reach consensus and improve data trustworthiness. Related codes were split, grouped together or removed to form broader agreed categories that minimised overlap, before moving to the deductive step with the TDF theoretical lens. Minor differences and discrepencies in initial data coding and category development were discussed with a third researcher (OL) and resolved by consensus. NVivo (qualitative analysis software) was used to support this process. This flexible reflective process helped maintained focus on the specific experiences of lifestyle behaviour change post-stroke. The TDF provided an overarching framework where the identified categories could be mapped deductively to its domains. Any identified categories that were unrelated to stroke secondary prevention were mapped to non-TDF themes.
Patient and public involvement (PPI)
A panel of stroke survivor champions worked alongside the research team and actively contributed to informing the research question and refining the study processes. This panel comprised five adults (N = 2 male; N = 3 female) with a variety of experiences along the stroke pathway. We adapted our protocol for recruitment and data collection based on their feedback, including developing more accessible recruitment materials in different formats. Consultation for example on the content and design of a participant information leaflet resulted in a more inclusive product which included an online oral and visual presentation. A schedule of questions prepared by the researcher as a focus group topic guide, was reviewed and refined in a roundtable consultation event to ensure the content reflected questions that were relevant to and used appropriate language for people with lived experience of stroke. The topic guide and focus group discussion was piloted in a practice run with the panel, prior to data collection, and refined to incorporate visual props/prompts during the discussions and ensured timing of discussions was short enough to minimise fatigue. The unique insights from one champion (N = 1, female, JH) helped with the interpretation and reporting of the findings. The revised Guidance for Reporting Involvement of Patients and the Public (GRIPP)2 (short-form) [42] (Additional file 5) was used to report PPI influence in the study.
Results
Participant characteristics
Eight focus groups were conducted between September and December 2022. Thirty eight individuals (N = 35 stroke survivors and N = 3 family members/informal carers) participated (Table 1). No participant who signed and returned the consent form withdrew from the study. Each focus group constituted between 2 and 10 participants; the average number per group was 5 participants. All groups comprised a mix of men and women. The focus group that was co-facilitated by a speech and language therapist for participants with aphasia had 10 participants.
Themes
A total of sixty-nine initial meaning units were identified after initial codes were converged or split and agreed between the two independent coders following discussions. Twenty-two broad categories were identified from these meaning units, mapping to ten of the fourteen possible TDF domains. Some meaning units mapped to more than one category and some categories mapped to two TDF domains. No categories that were identified mapped to the TDF domains addressing Skills, Optimism, Intentions, Environmental Context and Resources. The remaining categories identified mapped to one non-TDF theme related to life after stroke addressing general recovery, physical rehabilitation and roles and responsibilities rather than specific stroke secondary prevention health behaviours. No category identified mapped to a construct for behaviour change that was not addressed by the TDF domains. Results are presented for each TDF domain identified with quotes illustrating the behavioural mediators elucidated by participants (Table 2).
TDF domains
Knowledge
The knowledge domain of the TDF addresses individuals’ understanding and awareness of health behaviour/s. Participants across all groups reported understanding the concept of a healthy lifestyle in general and discussed health behaviours necessary to reduce the risk of stroke/stroke recurrence (healthy diet, regular exercise, not smoking, safe alcohol consumption). When explored in more detail, some participants could personalise this knowledge and identify prior behaviours as contributory to their stroke and that required action going forwards:
“But I was drinking too much beer, you know, and I consider that and the smoking certainly contributed to the, the stroke” (Participant 4 (male), FG4).
And
“So cholesterol, I’d never thought about it, I’d eat the fat off bacon! But when I, then had the stroke. And this is now obvious, this is what caused the stroke…. the lifestyle.” (Participant 2 (male), FG1).
Others focussed in on aspects of a healthy lifestyle such as fitness as protective against stroke but then struggled to rationalise these beliefs in the face of their own stroke:
“I never thought I’d have a stroke… up every morning at half 7, to swim. Walked every day, walked about for an hour every day. Did the garden. Did the hoovering [vacuuming] and stuff at home like that. Now you can’t put it down to health because I thought I was very fit” (Participant 1 (male), FG5).
Some participants highlighted their knowledge and understanding of stroke risk were largely based on the people they knew who had experienced stroke and centred on non-modifiable factors such as age:
“because like, the only person I ever knew to have a stroke was my grandfather. Even the night I was having my stroke, I was like! There’s no way I’m having a stroke… I’m a young one” (Participant 1 (female), FG4).
Consistently across groups, participants referenced many unmet needs related to understanding secondary prevention. They reported receiving little or no individualised information that helped them to better understanding their stroke risk when they were in hospital
“well, I never knew what kind of stroke I had or anything. I was never told…they told me absolutely nothing.”(Participant 2 (female), FG3).
And
“Well, yeah. How to prevent it? What’s his cholesterol, blood pressure, all that kind of thing that you wouldn’t have known before this”(Participant 10 (female), FG7).
Participants provided insights into the knowledge they required to enact lifestyle changes, and again highlighted unmet information and support needs. Participants regularly discussed in their groups how they (and their families) had to work things out for themselves:
“I was told that I had to lose weight, that I had to eat healthy, that I had to manage my cholesterol, to manage my blood pressure, I had to exercise more… But what I found was nobody told us how!…and then we figured it all out ourselves! but nobody actually told us”(Participant 1 (female), FG4).
Across the groups, participants acknowledged the role voluntary organisations play in providing stroke secondary prevention information and how again these supports were not signposted for them. They further highted that the timing of accessing these services was sub-optimal:
“And I mean they’re all very good (lifestyle talks and about blood pressure), but they’re all nearly too little, too much, too late. You know, we need these things beforehand” (Participant 2 (female), FG5).
And
“my daughter spotted that thing about Headways (voluntary organisation), and she said I’m going to [call], other than that I would be left sitting in the sitting room…”(Participant 1 (female), FG3).
When reflecting on the optimal time to receive targeted secondary prevention information and whether the message should extend beyond the person to the wider family members, participants differed in opinions. Results highlighted the complexity of the post-acute stage and an overlap between the TDF Knowledge domain and that of Memory, Attention, Decision Process in relation to processing information and information overload during the acute phase of stroke:
“There’s so much happening around you, you’re not really absorbing. It’s only coming later that it kind of dawns on you…” (Participant 2 (male), FG1).
And
“She got a lot of information, but she couldn’t… It went over her head.” (Participant 3 (female), FG7).
And
“I was getting a lot of information in the hospital and I couldn’t process it because I was tired and my brain wasn’t working” (Participant 1 (female), FG6).
Social/professional role & identity
This TDF domain addresses the influence of one’s social or professional role on health behaviours. During group discussions about risk reduction after stroke, the construct of self-identity was noted to play an evident role, notably for individuals who perceived themselves and their roles pre stroke to be essential for their healthy lifestyle:
“I was a postman for 38 years and I always had a sort of a routine. I went to bed at 10 o’clock almost every night, give or take. I’d get up at 6 or 5 rather and I’d start work at 6” (Participant 4 (male), FG2).
One family member/informal carer shared the effect his social role (as grandad) within the family helped her husband build and maintain healthy habits:
“Tommy (not real name) had stopped walking by himself! We were looking after our grandson one day and he said Granddad, will you bring me down to the (football) pitch. And you did…. And then I said, if you can do it with (grandson), you can do it yourself. So most days he takes a short walk down around the field himself.” (Participant 10 (female), FG7).
Others described the impact of perceived loss of their identity or feeling like a different person after stroke e.g. when talking about emotional self-regulation and their ability to engage with a healthy lifestyle:
“I just miss the old me. I know my sense of humour is different and I know it’s in there. But to get it out. I just can’t.” (Participant 1 (female), FG3).
This loss of identity was particularly noted when communication difficulties were a consequence of the stroke, evident in the focus group, co-facilitated by a speech and language therapist for participants with aphasia. Discussions about receiving information and guidance about stroke risk, resulted in consensus amongst the group members about how people talk around them, not to them directly and how this made them feel invisible or dismissed:
“people talk, talk around you” (Participant 4 (male)) “Yeah, yeah, that’s exactly it….”(Participant 6 (female)) “Talk down to me. That’s true”…(Participant 4 (male)) “And he said, don’t know what you’re saying, or what you said”(Participant 3 (female)) (FG7).
Beliefs about capabilities
This TDF domain encompasses an individual’s self-confidence and perception of their ability to perform health behaviours. During focus group discussions, participants talked about their confidence or lack of confidence in their own ability to adopt healthy behaviours, and how this influenced their attitudes towards particular behaviours, for example, being physically active:
“I think the more you can do something for yourself, the more confidence it gives you… I just started very slowly down the road and back to the house, until I could go around the block.” (Participant 2 (male), FG2).
And maintaining or not, a healthy diet:
“I was sweet eater, and diabetic. Now you said look, you don’t need them. And I said, Oh yeah, but I have to have something there in case I do want to.” (Participant 3 (female), FG2).
Some participants also described their struggles dealing with addictions, primarily in relation to smoking cessation after stroke. They discussed how not having another stroke was a strong motivating factor and how formal support services contributed to their engagement and perceived ability to stop:
“she said you keep smoking here, you’ll finish, you’ll get another stroke 10 years time and you’ll finish up in a wheelchair…. So the following Friday I stopped smoking.” (Participant 1 (male), FG6).
A family member/informal carer verbalised her lack of confidence in her husband’s ability to adopt healthy eating into his lifestyle by himself. She went on to discuss how she perceived it as her responsibility to provide a healthy diet to enable his capacity for risk reducing behaviours:
“I had to come to the realisation that I was responsible. It was up to me as the person who does the shopping to make sure we have a healthy diet.” (Participant 4 (female family member), FG1).
Beliefs about consequences
This TDF domain refers to the perceptions of positive or negative outcomes associated with health behaviour/s and the expectations individuals hold relating to their actions. Here focus group discourse largely concentrated on the negative consequences associated with sustained unhealthy behaviours and discussed how fear was their motivator for adopting better health behaviours:
“because I’m so scared of something happening. But I do tell myself this (gym class) will prevent you from ever going through what you went through again” (Participant 1 (female), FG4).
And
“I’m more conscious of food… Is it going to impact on stroke recurring.” (Participant 2 (male), FG1).
And
“the biggest factor I think, which is all [of us] is we don’t want another stroke and we don’t want to die.” (Participant 3 (male), FG6).
In most of the discussion groups, participants described their beliefs about the benefits of adopting good lifestyle behaviours on health as a general concept but did not personalise this to their own behaviours:
“The more active you are, in my opinion, the longer chance you have of living.” (Participant 1 (male), FG5).
And
“keep away from unhealthy food. Eat healthy food. Go for walks every day” (Participant 6 (female), FG4).
However one participant reported not caring about the consequences, related for example to a healthy diet:
“I don’t think about something that’s healthy or unhealthy. If I want to eat steak, I eat steak, I don’t care if it’s high, in anything. You know, I don’t think about those things…” (Participant 4 (male), FG4).
This attitude was not shared widely in this group, nor did it arise in other FGDs.
Reinforcement
This TDF domain acknowledges the external factors that can encourage or discourage health behaviours. In relation to enabling change and increasing the probability of maintaining new health behaviours, participants reported the importance of receiving external reminders and regular reinforcement of participants’ active and positive health behaviours:
“it’s no harm to keep reminding us about these things, because you can get complacent.” (Participant 2 (female), FG5).
Participants also welcomed external provision of rules to follow as an important mediator for maintaining behaviour change:
“They’ve done so much work on me, it’d be foolish to ignore it, you have to follow the rules, like that’s it.” (Participant 3 (male), FG6).
However, when external factors dictated a health behaviour without respecting personal autonomy, this was very poorly received. For example, one participant recalled being told he was no longer a smoker in hospital:
“The consultant, came round to see me in the hospital and he said… You were a smoker… He just closed the book like that (demonstrated closing a book) He walked away.” (Participant 2 (male), FG1).
Goals
This TDF domain addresses goal setting and action planning to achieve the necessary outcomes for adopting healthy behaviours. Notably, as a behaviour change construct it did not arise in many of the discussion groups. One participant did explain how she has set an activity goal for herself to track her steps, but how due to her irregular gait she cannot use digital counters to facilitate this:
“I set myself up a target, I try to walk 2000 steps every day. So when I’m walking from A to B I count my steps.” (Participant 2 (female), FG5).
Another participant described how her goal to return to playing golf motivated her to stay connected with an active social life, supported her mental health and eventually enabled her to achieve her goal:
“I played a lot golf, I’d a good social life, plenty of friends. that was a saviour from when I had my stroke, because I was able to go back to that, even though I couldn’t play golf, just meeting the people really brought me out of myself again and that gave me a goal, even though it took 2 years to get back to that [golf” (Participant 3 (female), FG8).
Memory, attention, decision process
This TDF domain addresses the cognitive factors that affect the adoption of health behaviours. During the focus group discussions, many participants reported how their memory and cognition were affected by stroke. This was most evident in the initial period post-stroke and at hospital discharge when much of the information relating to risk reducing behaviours is provided by health professionals:
“[re stroke information] I knew nothing. I don’t even remember going home from hospital.” (Participant 1 (female), FG3).
And
“[my wife] is capable of asking. Yes. But I couldn’t remember,” (Participant 4 (male), FG7).
Their ability to make sense of the information that was provided about stroke and secondary prevention and process it for behaviour change was also discussed in the groups as being challenged:
“there’s so much happening around you, plus the fact I think that the sheer shock of having the stroke, the realisation,” (Participant 6 (male), FG1).
Mental fatigue was identified as an important barrier in maintaining attention and concentration when addressing lifestyle-related risk factors:
“I was getting a lot of information in the hospital and I couldn’t process it because… I was tired and my brain wasn’t working” (Participant 2 (female), FG6).
Social influences
This TDF domain acknowledges the impact of others on individuals’ health behaviour/s and includes social norms and support. In all focus group discussions, participants described the influence social groups/networks and family had on their subsequent knowledge, understanding and engagement in healthy behaviours. One aspect of this related to their perceived lack of information provision from health professionals:
“My kids were good. They’re in their 30s and 40s and my eldest daughter, she’s kind of well up on it… so it was her told me what I can and can’t eat.” (Participant 1 (female), FG3).
Relying on friends or family for support or help with healthy behaviours and associated activities elicited some positive experiences:
“My sister brings me now. She comes down on a Friday and takes me shopping” (Participant 3 (female), FG2).
Negative experiences were also discussed with support for risk reduction leading to potential conflicts within family relationships and dynamics:
“… and sometimes it could be a little bit tetchy because [my husband] he likes loads of sweet things” (Participant 4 (female family member), FG1).
And
“my head doesn’t work so I have… beans all the time but… Just leave me alone… if I want to eat it [beans] every day… Leave It, that’s it.” (Participant 6 (female), FG7).
Being a member of a community stroke support group was perceived as a very positive influence and a motivator to engage in healthy activities:
“And the interaction with people with the same condition in the past, I swear to God. It’s like you know them for years. [Yeah].” (Participant 6 (male), FG1).
And
“The exercise is better because you get to know the people and you feel you’re getting the benefit out of exercise and you sort of look forward to it. Meet the people and do the exercise.” (Participant 1 (male), FG5).
And
“the heart foundation has done it’s part too. It’s had a dietitian on a few times, to its stroke groups, and you know, if they keep that up, some of it will eventually stick” (Participant 5 (male), FG8).
Family/friends and support groups were credited with supporting and encouraging mood management as a health behaviour:
“just meeting the people, really, really brought me out of myself again because you can, with stroke, you can go in to yourself” (Participant 3 (female), FG8).
And
“someone even just to say your brain is healing, you’re going to go through a lot of emotional changes. Talk to someone. This is the number to get you sorted” (Participant 2 (female), FG6).
However, often during the group discussions participants reported how they found out about community supports through informal networks, word of mouth or simply by chance:
“At some stage we got information there was a stroke group, health group in the area. So after further inquiries we discovered this place and have been coming since,” (Participant 1 (male), FG2).
Emotion
This TDF domain recognises the role emotions can play in influencing health behaviour/s. Discussions on risk reducing activities in the focus groups tended to centre around participants’ emotional reactions to having a stroke and how this impacted on their general acceptance and coping abilities. Participants discussed negative emotions related to acceptance of the stroke itself:
“It took me a long time to accept it and I’d be really angry. You know it, it was not depressed, but more anger. And possibly I would have cried once or twice at, at night, grieving for myself, I guess.” (Participant 6 (male), FG1).
And
“because I get up in the morning and I know when it’s time for crying. My tummy tells me… I could be out walking, and people have seen me crying and stopped and said what’s wrong with me? And I say, ah you know, just a headache or something like that…” (Participant 3 (male), FG3).
The effect of emotions on participating in healthy activities was also evident. Negative affect post-stroke was commonly acknowledged as a limiting factor to healthy lifestyle engagement. Having a positive outlook or attitude was reported as enabling and motivating towards healthy activities:
“So it’s all to do with your own mental attitude, I think. Just get up and go and get to do what you can” (Participant 1 (male), FG5).
It was evident that some participants recognised mood as a risk factor for recurrence and had adopted strategies to self-regulate mood as health behaviours:
“I had a tape to calm me down, used to listen to that. You know the sort of thing just to be calm in yourself, calm and relaxed.” (Participant 4 (male), FG2).
And
“I know when I get stressed, everyone knows… but then I just walk away and go and sit down. I say don’t talk to me for a while, I know I just manage my breathing. And it only takes a couple of minutes and then I’m back to normal.” (Participant 1 (female), FG3).
Behavioural regulation
This TDF domain addresses individuals’ ability to self-regulate and control their health behaviour/s. During group discourse, participants highlighted how they plan and monitor their lifestyle habits in order to stay on track. Routine was considered an important component to sustained behaviour change:
“Now when I wake up in the morning, get up and get dressed and get out for a walk and if I don’t do that I just sit all day, but I have to get up and get out straight away.” (Participant 2 (female), FG3).
Others described ways of monitoring and keeping track of their new behaviours:
“Adjustments, in the beginning you have to train the mind for regulating everything. But now it’s automatic” (Participant 5 (male), FG1).
And
“I keep an eye on the weight, maybe every day and If I see it going up I [cut back]” (Participant 1 (male), FG2).
This TDF domain was extended by participants beyond the first person to include family in monitoring and regulating behaviours:
“They bought me a Fitbit and I make sure to get me 10,000 steps in everyday and they check.” (Participant 1 (female), FG3).
And
“it is balanced because we have discovered that you can buy a sealer and we can get a bottle of wine and it will do us three nights.” (Participant 4 (female family member, FG1).
Patient and public involvement (PPI)
The analysis of study findings and interpretation of key messages was enriched through the unique insights from one PPI champion (JH). This level of research participation helped to highlight the vulnerability stroke survivors feel on the realisation of the risk of recurrence and how this can have both positive and negative impacts on lifestyle behaviours. Our champion recognised the “very obvious knowledge gap” after stroke and the importance of mental health as a lifestyle factor. Memories of her own experience were triggered which helped confirm our behavioural mediators during mapping. By providing a different perspective and identifying with the participants, JH helped lend credibility to our findings.
Discussion
Adopting and sustaining healthy behaviours after stroke is challenging, influenced by many elements from individual needs and preferences to external community and environmental factors [43, 44]. This qualitative study explored the real-world, lived experiences of adopting healthy lifestyle behaviours after stroke to reduce future risk. The study findings, examined under a comprehensive framework (TDF), allow a better understanding of the theoretical constructs that influence real world secondary prevention risk reducing behaviours. The results generated contribute important information that has relevance not only for prevention in cardiovascular conditions, but for health behaviour change processes that have wider utilisation in health promotion. Engagement with risk reducing behaviour change was largely discussed as a self-directed activity where people generally figured things out themselves. Participants were open to and welcomed additional direction and support to build knowledge and ability to make and sustain lifestyle changes however. The TDF provided a comprehensive lens through which to view the different influences or mediating factors for health behaviours after stroke. All mediators identified during the focus group discussions were exhaustively covered by this framework. No new constructs for behaviour change were identified despite adopting an inductive approach to data analysis in the first instance [33, 45]. Results, which mapped to ten of the fourteen possible TDF domains [45], help to better understand what works well, what doesn’t work well and where gaps to proactively reduce all modifiable, lifestyle related risk factors through behaviour change still exist after stroke.
Knowledge (e.g. nature of stroke, risk factors and lifestyle-related factors) was identified by participants in this study as a key construct for behaviour change. Qualitative studies previously highlighted the importance of information provision for addressing lifestyle risks after stroke [19, 25,26,27, 46], with quantitative evidence further supporting interactive and tailored information with feedback as effective in improving health behaviours after stroke [24, 47,48,49]. During focus group discussions, information provision received in the acute hospital setting primarily, could be described as passive provision. In contrast, active information provision, with opportunities for clarification and reinforcement, has been proven to improve stroke-survivor knowledge and reduce anxiety and depression after stroke [50]. While the current study identified that participants could easily discuss what constitutes a healthy lifestyle, many lacked personalised knowledge to relate their own lifestyle-related risk factors, and the Intentions behaviour change domain was not apparent when discussing their lifestyle and risk reducing activities. This lack of knowledge and awareness of personal unhealthy behaviours has been associated with poor intention to change behaviour after TIA or ischaemic stroke previously [15]. While knowledge is an important construct for behaviour change, information provision alone has been shown to be ineffective in affecting positive behaviour change [7, 17]. Additional factors need to be explored in tandem with active information provision to understand when and how people change their behaviours to lower their stroke risk [51]. Notably in this study, no behavioural change discussion categories mapped to the TDF domain of Skills, where proficiency is acquired through practice, or that of Intentions indicating a personal and conscious decision to perform a behaviour. Most participants valued reminders, reinforcements, and rules to observe for maintaining healthy behaviours, mapping to the Reinforcement and Behavioural Regulation constructs of the TDF. These may all be important components by which to complement knowledge and addresses the currently identified lack of “how to” identified in this study. As constructs, they can be incorporated easily into multimodal secondary prevention interventions comprising targeted health education and behaviour change activities focused on lifestyle and psychosocial health, as recommended in international guidelines [7, 8] and supported by high level evidence synthesis [19].
This study identified health beliefs as being important after stroke in adopting lifestyle changes. Illness perceptions after stroke have previously been explored using the theory of planned behaviour [26] and the health beliefs model [51]. When addressed, appropriately challenged, misperceptions corrected and evidence of necessity provided, a positive impact on health behaviours after stroke is evident [52, 53]. The predominant evidence available in this area addresses medication adherence which interestingly did not arise in focus group discussions as a risk reducing health behaviour. Personal health beliefs and perceived ability to change behaviour/s were evident and often overlapped with participants’ understanding of the benefits of healthy lifestyle behaviours. For many participants in the study, previous roles that promoted a healthy lifestyle, or strongly perceiving they had a healthy lifestyle prior to stroke (mapping to the TDF Social/professional role & identity) positively influenced their engagement in healthy activities, mirroring Antonovsky’s theory on salutogenesis where the way people view their life has a positive (or negative influence) on their health and coping ability [54]. It is interesting in this context to note that categories identified in this study that mapped to Beliefs about consequences in relation to health behaviours tended to the negative or punitive aspect (e.g. if you don’t do this you will have another stroke) and notably no categories mapped to the TDF domain of Optimism. The paradigm shift towards more positive messaging focusing on behaviours that support psychological well-being in cardiovascular disease needs to extend to the stroke secondary prevention population [55].
Psychosocial stress and depression are both recognised risk factors for stroke [1] and it is well acknowledged that stroke survivors often experience emotional changes, such as depression, anxiety, anger, or apathy [56]. Mapping to the Emotions domain of the TDF, psychosocial challenges and emotional responses encountered after stroke were strongly evident in group discussions where participants discussed how they negatively impacted their ability to engage in health promoting activities. However, the direct association between mood and stroke recurrence was more tenuous. This was an aspect that resonated with our PPI representative who felt strongly that this must be better recognised and addressed along the stroke care pathway. Psychosocial interventions after stroke that address emotional self-regulation as a risk-reducing health behaviour have been addressed at systematic review level [56,57,58,59,60,61]. A best evidence synthesis across these reviews identifies moderate certainty evidence [62] supporting psychological talk therapies to reduce post-stroke depression, whilst low certainty evidence supports multimodal interventions to reduce anxiety and psychological distress [19]. No focus group participant specifically referred to availing of either of these strategies when talking about managing their mood. Rather, participants’ experiences related to self-taught strategies, congruent with evidence from mindfulness-based cognitive therapy [63, 64] to address self-regulation of low mood after stroke [24].
Our findings emphasise social interactions and supports as a commonly cited and positive mediator for risk reducing behaviours after stroke. Social influence and behaviour change “occurs when an individual’s opinions, emotional states and behaviours are affected by others” [65], as identified in the current study. Several studies report social (family, friends, community) supports as predictive of community participation and functional recovery after stroke as well as enabling healthy behaviour engagement [32, 66,67,68]. Peer support groups [69, 70] and group-based programmes including cardiac rehabilitation after stroke [37] have been found to enable peers to provide and receive relevant emotional support and practical information. In this study voluntary stroke support groups proved an important vehicle in promoting positive health and wellness messaging and were seen as a shared space for obtaining new knowledge, understanding and engagement in healthy behaviours, with the added value of camaraderie without having to explain themselves amongst peers. Many participants expressed a need for ongoing reinforcement and refreshed guidance and information on health behaviours. Structured peer support programmes may provide opportunity for this reinforcement of messaging as well as goal setting and action planning, which was not optimised by our study participants in their current stroke pathways.
Studies to date have not explored secondary prevention needs after stroke directly mapped to behaviour change constructs, as encompassed in the TDF. Participants in the current study highlighted unmet practical, psychosocial and educational needs related to changing health behaviours. They further identified items that did not facilitate health behaviour change after stroke. For example, participants reported a need for clear, accurate, personalised and timely information from reliable sources e.g. healthcare professionals. Much of the information they received however, was in the acute phase of stroke and was not processed well by them, as previously evidenced in the literature [26] where the subacute, active rehabilitation phase is potentially more optimal [71].
A major health event such as stroke can motivate individuals to adopt a healthier lifestyle [65]. This is known as the ‘teachable window’ [72], a period of psychosocial sensemaking that presents an opportunity for providing lifestyle counselling to change health behaviours. However, even when receptive to receiving lifestyle information after stroke, the pedagogical approach taken may still play a significant role to successful adoption of health behaviours. Evidence suggests, as also identified in this study, that up to 80% of information delivered by health care practitioners is immediately forgotten [73] and recall of the information is often incorrect [74]. Guidance on optimal person-centred health educational strategies to employ for stroke secondary prevention remains a significant knowledge gap. However strategies such as checking patient understanding, as used in the teach-back method [75, 76] for example, have shown to be effective across a range of healthcare settings and clinical populations for learning-related and health-related outcomes [77]. This further intersects with the domain of Memory, Attention and Decision Process where cognitive difficulties, stress, mood and fatigue problems after stroke have negative implications that include information processing speed, attention, and recall, with consequences that extend to health status and quality of life [78,79,80,81]. These particular barriers require practical strategies to counteract them [82] that must also be harnessed for health behaviour change also, alongside more careful consideration about the timing of risk reduction education and the need for reinforcement. Drawing from the evidence garnered in this study, greater attention to appropriately timed intervention and inclusion of these theoretical constructs could support healthy lifestyle after stroke.
Goal setting and action planning are essential components of theoretically informed behaviour change [83]. The stark absence of information and support about how to affect behaviour change discussed in this study was highlighted by the absence of the Skills domain of the TDF in current secondary prevention experiences. When considered together with the underutilisation of mediators for change such as Goals and the absence of Intentions and Environmental Context and Resources domains, current experiences of secondary prevention actions after stroke suggest a lack of structured assistance to promote, motivate and support behaviour change. Effective self-management [22, 84] and enhanced secondary prevention intervention trials post-stroke employing these domains have demonstrated improved overall risk factor control [85]. The absence of the TDF domains of Skills, Optimism, Intentions, Environmental Context and Resources in our mapping process, represent missed opportunities to optimise health behaviour change, likely reflecting a lack of availability of theoretically informed programmes for secondary prevention in stroke care pathways [24] alongside more negative health messaging pervasive in stroke care [86]. These significant gaps in theory-informed supports currently provided, signal opportunity to better direct effective and targeted interventions in the future.
Strengths and limitations
This study adds to the literature exploring healthy lifestyle adoption after stroke. Its unique focus on experiences of stroke secondary prevention behaviours, examined using a comprehensive theoretical framework of behaviour change and their constructs, has direct relevance and lessons for secondary prevention interventions and stroke support services development and delivery. A strength and novelty of the study is that it explores the person’s lived experience of adopting health behaviours after stroke, examining the behavioural mediators that influence their choices. Embedded PPI strengthened the conduct and insights drawn from the study. The study strived to include a diverse sample of participants and maximised inclusivity by facilitating a communications group with post-stroke aphasia, a broad range of ages and an urban/rural mix. However, as the recruitment approach relied on members of support groups, it is likely that they had more information and support than others who choose not to join such groups. They are also more likely to be individuals who gravitate towards group-based activities. Most participants in the individual focus groups were known to each other as members of the support group which may have impacted (positively or negatively) on the discussions. The primary author had worked in both organisations a number of years before the study was conducted which may have blurred the insider-outsider boundaries of being a clinician and a researcher. Whilst an inductive deductive approach to analysis was applied using the TDF, different results may have emerged if a different framework or approach to data analysis was used.
Risk of stroke and stroke recurrence are influenced not only by individual behaviour but also by environmental and social determinants of health [87]. Whilst the inter- and intrapersonal aspects for behaviour change were the focus of this research, it is important to acknowledge the complex influences income, education, social isolation, ethnicity and environmental factors contribute to disparities in health behaviours and warrant consideration in the context of behaviour change.
Patient and public involvement (PPI).
Reflecting on PPI as an integral part of this study, the stroke PPI panel and co-author (JH) were involved in a range of activities which provided key insights and understanding for the rest of the research team. Trouble-shooting prior to recruitment and data collection ensured a smoother transition. Overall, champions reported their involvement as partners as a positive experience and felt supported in the process. Understanding the key messages generated by this study were enriched through the interpretation of findings from our co-author’s perspective. JH who contributed to this process provided personalised insights into the theoretical constructs that influence lifestyle behaviour and checked the validity of the conclusions from a stroke survivor’s perspective. She particularly empathised with the importance of emotional support as a contributor to secondary prevention and knowledge gaps beyond the acute phase. Planning PPI activities from the outset made this level of involvement possible and was facilitated in this study by an already established and well-functioning stroke PPI panel and inclusion of an experienced co-researcher. PPI related challenges encountered related to the quantity of information to process for PPI participants alongside their competing work and life demands. However, the quality of their engagement throughout was excellent, motivated in part by the study’s potential to positively contribute to future intervention development and service provision for those coming after.
Conclusions
This qualitative study explored the real-world, lived experiences of adopting healthy lifestyle behaviours after stroke. The TDF proved a valuable tool to link stroke survivors’ secondary prevention experiences and unmet needs with identified constructs for behaviour-change. Participants reported largely self-directed activities where domains of Knowledge, Beliefs and Social influences played a key role in lifestyle behaviour. Identity, Emotions and Memory domains intersect strongly with their ability to adopt and sustain healthy behaviour change. Despite most participants not setting explicit goals, they still tracked and adjusted their actions. The absence of domains Skills, Optimism, Intentions and Environmental context & resources likely speaks to the lack of structured theory-based programmes providing opportunities to support risk recognition by linking to goal setting and action planning, practice, and environmental prompts to promote and motivate behaviour change. The absence of the Optimism domain in particular, suggests a current lack of emphasis on positive health messaging. These findings require careful consideration to address missed opportunities to better direct additional strategies for effective targeted intervention development.
Data availability
The raw data transcripts generated during the current study are not publicly available to ensure anonymity of participants but aggregated anonymised data are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
References
O’Donnell MJ, Chin SL, Rangarajan S, Xavier D, Liu L, Zhang H, et al. Global and regional effects of potentially modifiable risk factors associated with acute stroke in 32 countries (INTERSTROKE): a case-control study. Lancet. 2016;388(10046):761–75.
Feigin VL, Stark BA, Johnson CO, Roth GA, Bisignano C, Abady GG, et al. Global, regional, and National burden of stroke and its risk factors, 1990–2019: a systematic analysis for the global burden of disease study 2019. Lancet Neurol. 2021;20(10):795–820.
Mohan KM, Wolfe CDA, Rudd AG, Heuschmann PU, Kolominsky-Rabas PL, Grieve AP. Risk and cumulative risk of stroke recurrence. Stroke. 2011;42(5):1489–94.
Talelli P, Greenwood RJ, Review. Recurrent stroke: where do we stand with the secondary prevention of noncardioembolic ischaemic strokes? Ther Adv Cardiovasc Dis. 2008;2(5):387–405.
Campbell BCV, Khatri P, Stroke. Lancet. 2020;396(10244):129–42.
Gladstone DJ, Lindsay MP, Douketis J, Smith EE, Dowlatshahi D, Wein T, et al. Canadian stroke best practice recommendations: secondary prevention of stroke update 2020. Can J Neurol Sci. 2022;49(3):315–37.
Kleindorfer DO, Towfighi A, Chaturvedi S, Cockroft KM, Gutierrez J, Lombardi-Hill D et al. 2021 Guideline for the prevention of stroke in patients with stroke and transient ischemic attack; A guideline from the American Heart Association/American Stroke Association. Stroke (1970). 2021;52(7):E364-E467.
Stroke Foundation. Clinical Guidelines for Stroke Management. 2023 [Available from: https://informme.org.au/guidelines/living-clinical-guidelines-for-stroke-management
Flach C, Muruet W, Wolfe CD, Bhalla A, Douiri A. Risk and secondary prevention of stroke recurrence: a population-base cohort study. Stroke. 2020;51(8):2435–44.
Bailey RR, Phad A, McGrath R, Haire-Joshu D. Prevalence of five lifestyle risk factors among US adults with and without stroke. Disabil Health J. 2019;12(2):323–7.
Lennon O, Hall P, Blake C. Predictors of adherence to lifestyle recommendations in stroke secondary prevention. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2021;18(9):4666.
Lawrence M, Asaba E, Duncan E, Elf M, Eriksson G, Faulkner J, et al. Stroke secondary prevention, a non-surgical and non-pharmacological consensus definition: results of a Delphi study. BMC Res Notes. 2019;12(1):823.
Davis R, Campbell R, Hildon Z, Hobbs L, Michie S. Theories of behaviour and behaviour change across the social and behavioural sciences: a scoping review. Health Psychol Rev. 2015;9(3):323–44.
English C, Ramage ER, Attia J, Bernhardt J, Bonevski B, Burke M et al. Secondary prevention of stroke. A telehealth-delivered physical activity and diet pilot randomized trial (ENAbLE-pilot). Int J Stroke. 2023:17474930231201360.
Brouwer-Goossensen D, den Hertog HM, Mastenbroek-de Jong MA, van Gemert-Pijnen L, Taal E. Patient perspectives on health-related behavior change after transient ischemic attack or ischemic stroke. Brain Behav. 2021;11(4):e01993.
Smith S, Parkinson J, Caitens T, Sanders A, Murphy L, Hamilton K. Promoting adherence to stroke secondary prevention behaviours by imparting behaviour change skills: protocol for a single-arm pilot trial of living well after stroke. BMJ Open. 2023;13(1):e068003.
Kelly MP, Barker M. Why is changing health-related behaviour so difficult? Public Health. 2016;136:109–16.
Redfern J, McKevitt C, Wolfe CDA. Development of complex interventions in stroke care. Stroke. 2006;37(9):2410–9.
Hall P, Lawrence M, Blake C, Lennon O. Interventions for behaviour change and self-management of risk in stroke secondary prevention: an overview of reviews. Cerebrovasc Dis. 2023.
Lawrence M, Kerr S, McVey MC, Godwin J. A systematic review of the effectiveness of secondary prevention lifestyle interventions designed to change lifestyle behaviour following stroke. JBI Libr Syst Rev. 2011;9(43):1782–827.
Lennon O, Galvin R, Smith K, Doody C, Blake C. Lifestyle interventions for secondary disease prevention in stroke and transient ischaemic attack: a systematic review. Eur J Prev Cardiol. 2014;21(8):1026–39.
Sakakibara BM, Kim AJ, Eng JJ. A systematic review and Meta-Analysis on Self-Management for improving risk factor control in stroke patients. Int J Behav Med. 2017;24(1):42–53.
Towfighi A, Markovic D, Ovbiagele B. Impact of a healthy lifestyle on all-cause and cardiovascular mortality after stroke in the USA. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2012;83(2):146–51.
Hall P, Lawrence M, Kroll T, Blake C, Matthews J, Lennon O. Reducing risk behaviours after stroke: an overview of reviews interrogating primary study data using the theoretical domains framework [Manuscript submitted for publication]. 2024.
Allison R, Evans PH, Kilbride C, Campbell JL. Secondary prevention of stroke: using the experiences of patients and carers to inform the development of an educational resource. Fam Pract. 2008;25(5):355–61.
Lawrence M, Kerr S, Watson H, Paton G, Ellis G. An exploration of lifestyle beliefs and lifestyle behaviour following stroke: findings from a focus group study of patients and family members. BMC Fam Pract. 2010;11(1):97.
Lennon OC, Doody C, Ni Choisdealbh C, Blake C. Barriers to healthy-lifestyle participation in stroke: consumer participation in secondary prevention design. Int J Rehabil Res. 2013;36(4):354–61.
Yuki T, Kudo M. Factors related to continuation of health behaviours among stroke survivors. J Jpn Phys Ther Assoc. 2011;14(1):1–11.
Jamison J, Graffy J, Mullis R, Mant J, Sutton S. Barriers to medication adherence for the secondary prevention of stroke: a qualitative interview study in primary care. Br J Gen Pract. 2016;66(649):e568–76.
Nicholson SL, Donaghy M, Johnston M, Sniehotta FF, van Wijck F, Johnston D, et al. A qualitative theory guided analysis of stroke survivors’ perceived barriers and facilitators to physical activity. Disabil Rehabil. 2014;36(22):1857–68.
Outermans J, Pool J, van de Port I, Bakers J, Wittink H. What’s keeping people after stroke from walking outdoors to become physically active? A qualitative study, using an integrated biomedical and behavioral theory of functioning and disability. BMC Neurol. 2016;16(1):137.
Lawrence M, Pringle J, Kerr S, Booth J. Stroke survivors’ and family members’ perspectives of multimodal lifestyle interventions for secondary prevention of stroke and transient ischemic attack: a qualitative review and meta-aggregation. Disabil Rehabil. 2016;38(1):11–21.
McGowan LJ, Powell R, French DP. How can use of the theoretical domains framework be optimized in qualitative research? A rapid systematic review. Br J Health Psychol. 2020;25(3):677–94.
Cane J, O’Connor D, Michie S. Validation of the theoretical domains framework for use in behaviour change and implementation research. Implement Sci. 2012;7(1):37.
Hanley B, Bradburn J, Barnes M, Evans C, Goodare H, Kelson M, et al. Involving the public in NHS public health, and social care research: briefing. notes for researchers: Involve; 2004.
Tong A, Sainsbury P, Craig J. Consolidated criteria for reporting qualitative research (COREQ): a 32-item checklist for interviews and focus groups. Int J Qual Health Care. 2007;19(6):349–57.
Lennon O, Crystal A, Kwan M, Tierney C, Gallagher A, Murphy S. Perspectives and experiences of cardiac rehabilitation after Stroke - A qualitative study. Healthcare. 2022;10(8):1579.
Kjörk EK, Gunnel C, Lundgren-Nilsson Å, Sunnerhagen KS. Experiences, needs, and preferences for follow-up after stroke perceived by people with stroke and healthcare professionals: A focus group study. PLoS ONE. 2019;14(10):e0223338.
Stalmeijer RE, McNaughton N, Van Mook WNKA. Using focus groups in medical education research: AMEE guide 91. Med Teach. 2014;36(11):923–39.
Gale NK, Heath G, Cameron E, Rashid S, Redwood S. Using the framework method for the analysis of qualitative data in multi-disciplinary health research. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2013;13(1):117.
Erlingsson C, Brysiewicz P. A hands-on guide to doing content analysis. Afr J Emerg Med. 2017;7(3):93–9.
Staniszewska S, Brett J, Simera I, Seers K, Mockford C, Goodlad S, et al. GRIPP2 reporting checklists: tools to improve reporting of patient and public involvement in research. BMJ. 2017;358:j3453.
Michie S, Fixsen D, Grimshaw JM, Eccles MP. Specifying and reporting complex behaviour change interventions: the need for a scientific method. Implement Science: IS. 2009;4(1):40.
Morris J, Oliver T, Kroll T, Macgillivray S. The importance of psychological and social factors in influencing the uptake and maintenance of physical activity after stroke: a structured review of the empirical literature. Stroke Res Treat. 2012;2012:195249.
Atkins L, Francis J, Islam R, O’Connor D, Patey A, Ivers N, et al. A guide to using the theoretical domains framework of behaviour change to investigate implementation problems. Implement Sci. 2017;12(1):77.
Faiz KW, Labberton AS, Thommessen B, Rønning OM, Barra M. Stroke-Related knowledge and lifestyle behavior among stroke survivors. J Stroke Cerebrovasc Dis. 2019;28(11):104359.
Evans-Hudnall GL, Stanley MA, Clark AN, Bush AL, Resnicow K, Liu Y, et al. Improving secondary stroke self-care among underserved ethnic minority individuals: a randomized clinical trial of a pilot intervention. J Behav Med. 2014;37(2):196–204.
Sit JW, Yip VY, Ko SK, Gun AP, Lee JS. A quasi-experimental study on a community-based stroke prevention programme for clients with minor stroke. J Clin Nurs. 2007;16(2):272–81.
Slark J, Khan M, Bentley P, Sharma P. Individual risk awareness intervention in stroke (IRAIS): a randomized controlled trial. J Cardiol. 2013;2(1):1017.
Crocker TF, Brown L, Lam N, Wray F, Knapp P, Forster A. Information provision for stroke survivors and their carers. Cochrane Database Syst Reviews. 2021(11).
Sullivan K, Waugh D. Toward the development of the cerebrovascular attitudes and beliefs scale (CABS): A measure of Stroke-Related health beliefs. Top Stroke Rehabil. 2007;14(3):41–51.
O’Carroll RE, Chambers JA, Dennis M, Sudlow C, Johnston M. Improving adherence to medication in stroke survivors: A pilot randomised controlled trial. Ann Behav Med. 2013;46(3):358–68.
Souter C, Kinnear A, Kinnear M, Mead G. Optimisation of secondary prevention of stroke: a qualitative study of stroke patients’ beliefs, concerns and difficulties with their medicines†. Int J Pharm Pract. 2014;22(6):424–32.
LindstrÖM B, Eriksson M. Contextualizing salutogenesis and Antonovsky in public health development. Health Promot Int. 2006;21(3):238–44.
Boehm JK, Kubzansky LD. The heart’s content: the association between positive psychological well-being and cardiovascular health. Psychol Bull. 2012;138(4):655–91.
Allida S, Cox KL, Hsieh CF, Lang H, House A, Hackett ML. Pharmacological, psychological, and non-invasive brain stimulation interventions for treating depression after stroke. Cochrane Database Syst Reviews. 2020(1).
Jeffares I, Merriman NA, Rohde D, McLoughlin A, Scally B, Doyle F, et al. A systematic review and meta-analysis of the effects of cardiac rehabilitation interventions on cognitive impairment following stroke. Disabil Rehabil. 2021;43(6):773–88.
Lawrence M, Kerr S, McVey C, Godwin J. The effectiveness of secondary prevention lifestyle interventions designed to change lifestyle behavior following stroke: summary of a systematic review. Int J Stroke. 2012;7(3):243–7.
Tao S, Geng Y, Li M, Ye J, Liu Z. Effectiveness of mindfulness-based stress reduction and mindfulness-based cognitive therapy on depression in poststroke patients-A systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. J Psychosom Res. 2022;163.
Wan X, Chau JPC, Mou H, Liu X. Effects of peer support interventions on physical and psychosocial outcomes among stroke survivors: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Int J Nurs Stud. 2021;121:104001.
Wang S-B, Wang Y-Y, Zhang Q-E, Wu S-L, Ng CH, Ungvari GS, et al. Cognitive behavioral therapy for post-stroke depression: A meta-analysis. J Affect Disord. 2018;235:589–96.
Guyatt GH, Oxman AD, Vist GE, Kunz R, Falck-Ytter Y, Alonso-Coello P, et al. GRADE: an emerging consensus on rating quality of evidence and strength of recommendations. BMJ. 2008;336(7650):924–6.
Wang X, Li J, Wang C, Lv J. The effects of mindfulness-based intervention on quality of life and poststroke depression in patients with spontaneous intracerebral hemorrhage in China. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2020;35(5):572–80.
Zhang YX, Hao ZW, Guo X. Clinical effect of mindfulness-based behavioral training on post-stroke depression. Chin J Cardiovasc Cerebrovasc Dis Integr Traditional Western Med. 2015;13(14):1679–81.
Kwasnicka D, Dombrowski SU, White M, Sniehotta F. Theoretical explanations for maintenance of behaviour change: a systematic review of behaviour theories. Health Psychol Rev. 2016;10(3):277–96.
Erler KS, Sullivan V, Mckinnon S, Inzana R. Social support as a predictor of community participation after stroke. Front Neurol. 2019;10.
Morris JH, Irvine LA, Dombrowski SU, McCormack B, Van Wijck F, Lawrence M. We walk: a person-centred, dyadic behaviour change intervention to promote physical activity through outdoor walking after stroke-an intervention development study. BMJ Open. 2022;12(6):e058563.
Woodman P, Riazi A, Pereira C, Jones F. Social participation post stroke: a meta-ethnographic review of the experiences and views of community-dwelling stroke survivors. Disabil Rehabil. 2014;36(24):2031–43.
Morris R, Morris P. Participants’ experiences of hospital-based peer support groups for stroke patients and carers. Disabil Rehabil. 2012;34(4):347–54.
Kessler D, Egan M, Kubina L-A. Peer support for stroke survivors: a case study. BMC Health Serv Res. 2014;14(1):256.
Hafsteinsdóttir TB, Vergunst M, Lindeman E, Schuurmans M. Educational needs of patients with a stroke and their caregivers: A systematic review of the literature. Patient Educ Couns. 2011;85(1):14–25.
Brust M, Gebhardt WA, van Bruggen S, Janssen V, Numans ME, Kiefte-de Jong JC. Making sense of a myocardial infarction in relation to changing lifestyle in the five months following the event: an interpretative phenomenological analysis. Soc Sci Med. 2023;338:116348.
McGuire LC. Remembering what the Doctor said: organization and adults’ memory for medical information. Exp Aging Res. 1996;22(4):403–28.
Keeble W, Cobbe SM. Patient recall of medication details in the outpatient clinic. Audit and assessment of the value of printed instructions requesting patients to bring medications to clinic. Postgrad Med J. 2002;78(922):479–82.
Schillinger D, Piette J, Grumbach K, Wang F, Wilson C, Daher C, et al. Closing the loop: physician communication with diabetic patients who have low health literacy. Arch Intern Med. 2003;163(1):83–90.
Shersher V, Haines TP, Sturgiss L, Weller C, Williams C. Definitions and use of the teach-back method in healthcare consultations with patients: A systematic review and thematic synthesis. Patient Educ Couns. 2021;104(1):118–29.
Talevski J, Wong Shee A, Rasmussen B, Kemp G, Beauchamp A. Teach-back: A systematic review of implementation and impacts. PLoS ONE. 2020;15(4):e0231350.
Husseini NE, Katzan IL, Rost NS, Blake ML, Byun E, Pendlebury ST, et al. Cognitive impairment after ischemic and hemorrhagic stroke: A scientific statement from the American heart association/american stroke association. Stroke. 2023;54(6):e272–91.
Gyawali P, Wong D, Hordacre B, Ong LK, English C. Editorial: stress, mood, and fatigue: tackling invisible Obstacles in stroke rehabilitation and recovery. Front Neurol. 2022;13:1121667.
Johansson B, Rönnbäck L. Mental fatigue and cognitive impairment after an almost neurological recovered stroke. ISRN Psychiatry. 2012;2012:686425.
Askari S, Harvey K, Sam-Odutola M. Patients experience of cognitive fatigue post-stroke: an exploratory study. Disabil Rehabil. 2023:1–8.
Tang EYH, Price C, Stephan BCM, Robinson L, Exley C. Impact of memory problems Post-stroke on patients and their family carers: A qualitative study. Front Med. 2020;7.
Bailey RR. Goal setting and action planning for health behavior change. Am J Lifestyle Med. 2019;13(6):615–8.
Satink T, Josephsson S, Zajec J, Cup EHC, de Swart BJM, Nijhuis-van der Sanden MWG. Self-management develops through doing of everyday activities—a longitudinal qualitative study of stroke survivors during two years post-stroke. BMC Neurol. 2016;16(1):221.
Gillham S, Endacott R. Impact of enhanced secondary prevention on health behaviour in patients following minor stroke and transient ischaemic attack: a randomized controlled trial. Clin Rehabil. 2010;24(9):822–30.
Anderson S, Marlett NJ. The Language of recovery: how effective communication of information is crucial to restructuring Post-Stroke life. Top Stroke Rehabil. 2004;11(4):55–67.
Yadav RS, Chaudhary D, Avula V, Shahjouei S, Azarpazhooh MR, Abedi V, et al. Social determinants of stroke hospitalization and mortality in united States’ counties. J Clin Med. 2022;11(14):4101.
Acknowledgements
The authors are grateful to the participants who willingly shared their experiences and thoughts. We wish to thank The Irish Heart Foundation and Croí Heart & Stroke for assistance with distributing information to eligible participants attending their stroke support groups. We also wish to acknowledge the valuable contribution of the PPI panel in this research.
Funding
This study is supported by the Irish Health Research Board (HRB) Collaborative Doctoral Award iPASTAR (improving Pathways for Acute Stroke and Rehabilitation) [CDA-2019-004]. The first author is a PhD scholar funded under this programme and OL is a named grant partner. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
PH and OL contributed to the concept/idea/research design. PH project managed the study, recruited participants, conducted the focus group discussions, transcribed, analysed and interpreted the data, drafted and revised the manuscript. OL provided supervision, reviewed the procedures for data generation, analysis and interpretation, and reviewed, revised and edited the manuscript. PP analysed and interpreted the data and reviewed reporting of findings. JH engaged with the methodology, research design, interpretation and reporting of findings. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Ethics approval and consent to participate
Ethical approval was obtained from University College Dublin Health Research Ethics Committee (LS-22-23-Hall-Lennon). The Irish Heart Foundation (IHF) and Croí Heart & Stroke Charity, representing key stakeholders, contributed as gatekeepers to participation, distributing information to eligible participants from community-based stroke support groups. All participants signed an informed consent to participate.
Consent for publication
Not applicable.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher’s note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Electronic supplementary material
Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License, which permits any non-commercial use, sharing, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if you modified the licensed material. You do not have permission under this licence to share adapted material derived from this article or parts of it. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Hall, P., da Cruz Peniche, P., Hickey, J. et al. Identifying mediators of healthy lifestyle adoption after stroke: a focus group study using a Theoretical Domains Framework guided analysis. BMC Neurol 25, 135 (2025). https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s12883-025-04144-1
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s12883-025-04144-1